Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of computer training and internet usage on cognitive abilities in older adults: a randomized controlled study

  • Original Articles
  • Published:
Aging Clinical and Experimental Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background and aims: According to the concepts of ‘use it or lose it’ and cognitive reserve, cognitively challenging activities may boost cognitive abilities in older adults. Using computers and the internet provides divergent cognitive challenges to older persons, and the positive effects of computers and internet use on the qualify of life have been found in earlier studies. Methods: We investigated whether prolonged guided computer use by healthy older adults (64–75) may be beneficial to cognitive ability in a randomized controlled study. The intervention consisted of brief training and subsequent use of a personal computer with an internet connection at home for a 12-month period. 191 participants were randomly assigned to three groups: Intervention, Training/No intervention, or No training/No intervention. A fourth group consisted of 45 participants with no interest in computer use. The effect of the intervention was assessed by a range of well-established cognitive instruments that probed verbal memory, information processing speed, and cognitive flexibility. Data were collected at baseline and after four and twelve months. Results: Intensive interaction with a personal computer with standard software applications had no effect on cognitive measures; no differences in changes in cognitive parameters over time were found between groups. Conclusion: Learning to use a computer and the internet does not benefit healthy, community-dwelling older adults with respect to many domains of cognitive functions. The implications of these findings for future studies that use cognitive challenge to counteract usual cognitive aging are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Craik FIM, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition, 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum: 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Schaie KW. The course of adult intellectual development. Am Psychol 1994; 49: 304–13.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Hultsch DF, Herzog C, Small BJ et al. Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 1999; 14: 245–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 2002; 287: 742–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 2003; 348: 2508–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bosma H, van Boxtel MP, Ponds RW et al. Engaged lifestyle and cognitive function in middle and old-aged, non-demented persons: a reciprocal association? Z Gerontol Geriatr 2002; 35: 575–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Christensen H, Korten A, Jorm AF et al. Activity levels and cognitive functioning in an elderly community sample. Age Ageing 1996; 25: 72–80.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Swaab DF. Brain aging and Alzheimer’s disease, “wear and tear” versus “use it or lose it”. Neurobiol Aging 1991; 12: 317–24.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Frick KM, Fernandez SM. Enrichment enhances spatial memory and increases synaptophys in levels in aged female mice. Neurobiol Aging 2003; 24: 615–26.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Milgram NW. Cognitive experience and its effect on age-dependent cognitive decline in beagle dogs. Neurochem Res 2003; 28: 1677–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. JINS 2002; 8: 448–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Stigsdotter A, Backman L. Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: Generalizability across tasks and individuals. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 1995; 50B: P134–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Valentijn SAM, van Hooren SAH, Bosma H et al. The effect of two types of memory training on subjective and objective memory performance in healthy individuals aged 55 years and older: a randomized controlled trial. Patient Educ Couns 2005; 57: 106–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L. Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging 1992; 7: 242–51.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2002; 288: 2271–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schaie KW, Willis SL. Can decline in adult intellectual functioning be reversed? Dev Psychobiol 1986; 22: 223–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Cody MJ, Dunn D, Hoppin S et al. Silver surfers: training and evaluating internet use among older adult learners. Communication Education 1999; 48: 269–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. White H, McConnell E, Clipp E et al. A randomized controlled trial of the psychosocial impact of providing internet training and access to older adults. Aging Ment Health 2002; 6: 213–21.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Strenge AJ, Walker N, Rogers WA. Searching the World Wide Web: can older adults get what they need? In Rogers WA, Fisk AD, eds. Human Factors Interventions for the Health Care of Older Adults. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2001: pp. 255–69.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Lin D-YM. Hypertext for the aged: effects of text topologies. Comput Human Behav 2003; 19: 201–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. McConatha D, McConatha JT, Dermigny R. The use of computer services to enhance the quality of life for long term care residents. Gerontologist 1994; 34: 553–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. McConatha JT, McConatha D, Deaner SL et al. A computer-based intervention for the education and therapy of institutionalized older adults. Educ Gerontol 1995; 21: 129–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–98.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Buchner A, Faul F, Erdfelder E. GPOWER: A priori-post hoc- and compromise power analyses for the Macintosh. In. 2.1 end., Series GPOWER: A priori-post hoc- and compromise power analyses for the Macintosh. Bonn, Germany: Bonn University, 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Jolles J, Houx PJ, van Boxtel MPJ et al, ed. Maastricht Aging Study: Determinants of cognitive aging Maastricht: Neuropsych Publishers, 1995.

  26. Moller JT, Cluitmans P, Rasmussen LS et al. Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. IS-POCD investigators, International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction. Lancet 1998; 351: 847–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. van der Elst W, van Boxtel MPJ, van Breukelen GPJ et al. Rey’s verbal learning test: Normative data for 1855 healthy participants aged 24–81 years and the influence of sex, education, and model of presentation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2005; 11: 290–302.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Houx PJ, Jolles J. Age-related decline of psychomotor speed: effects of age, brain health, sex and education. Percept Mot Skills 1993; 76: 195–211.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility — a model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev 1990; 97: 253–70.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. van der Elst W, van Boxtel MPJ, van Breukelen GPJ et al. The Letter Digit Substitution Test: normative data for 1858 healthy participants aged 24–81 from the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS): Influence of age, education and sex. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006; 28: 998–1009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Vink M, Jolles J. A new version of the Trail-Making Test as an information processing task. J Clin Neuropsychol 1985; 7: 162.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Houx PJ, Jolles J, Vreeling FW. Stroop interference: aging effects assessed with the Stroop Color-Word Test. Exp Aging Res 1993; 19: 316–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGerald P et al. The Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British J Clin Psychol 1982; 21: 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Merckelbach H, Muris P, Nijman H et al. Self-reported cognitive failures and neurotic symptomatology. Pers Individ Diff 1996; 20: 715–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kliegl R, Smith J, Baltes PB. Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Dev Psychol 1989; 25: 247–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Brooks JO, Friedman L, Pearman AM et al. Mnemonic training in older adults: effects of age, length of training, and type of cognitive pretraining. Int Psychogeriatr 1999; 11: 75–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Danowski JA, Sacks W. Computer communication and the elderly. Exp Aging Res 1980; 6: 125–35.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Sherer M. The impact of using personal computers on the lives of nursing home residents. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr 1996; 14: 13–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Czaja SJ, Sharit J. The influence of age and experience on the performance of a data entry task. In: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (HFES) 41st Annual Meeting. Vol. 1. The influence of age and experience on the performance of a data entry task. Albuquerque: HFES, 1997: pp. 144–7.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martin van Boxtel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Slegers, K., van Boxtel, M. & Jolles, J. Effects of computer training and internet usage on cognitive abilities in older adults: a randomized controlled study. Aging Clin Exp Res 21, 43–54 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03324898

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03324898

Keywords

Navigation