Skip to main content
Log in

Examining the linguistic coding differences hypothesis to explain individual differences in foreign language learning

  • Part IV Language Difficulties In Other Subgroups
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, it is suggested that foreign language learning problems result from difficulties with native language learning and hypothesized that difficulties with phonological processing may be the locus of foreign language learning difficulties for some poor foreign language learners. Evidence is described that supports these positions. It is argued that conceptualizing foreign language learning problems as alanguage problem allows researchers to more clearly specify deficits related to the learning of a foreign language. Research evidence which shows that good and poor foreign language learners exhibit significantly different levels of native language skill and phonological processing is summarized. Finally, potential challenges to my hypotheses as an explanation for foreign language learning problems are reviewed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Au, S. 1988. A critical appraisal of Gardner’s social-psychological theory of second language (L2) learning.Language Learning 38:75–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. 1983. Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection.Nature 301:419–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. 1985.Rhyme and Reason in Reading and Spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. 1991. The role of working memory in reading disability. InPhonological Processes in Literacy, eds. S. Brady and D. Shankweiler. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, K. 1988. Preschool language processing performance and later reading achievement. InPreschool Prevention of Reading Failure, eds. R. Masland and M. Masland. Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. 1962. The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. InTraining and Research in Education, ed. R. Glaser. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. 1990. Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: Then and now. InLanguage Aptitude Reconsidered, eds. T. Parry and C. Stansfield. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J., and Sapon S. 1959.Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT) Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. 1986. Speech production/phonological deficits in reading disordered children.Journal of Learning Disabilities 19:504–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S. 1989. Why poor readers misunderstand spoken sentences. InPhonology and Reading Disability, eds. D. Shankweiler and I. Liberman. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A., Nagy, W., and Hancin, B. 1993. Cross-Language transfer of phonemic awareness.Journal of Educational Psychology 85:453–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. 1987. Effects of printed language acquisition on speech. InLiteracy, Language, and Learning, eds. D. Olson, N. Torrence, and A. Hilyard. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrman, M. 1990. The role of personality type in adult language learning: An ongoing investigation. InLanguage Aptitude Reconsidered, eds. T. Parry and C. Stansfield. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrman, M., and Oxford, R. 1995. Cognition plus: Correlates of language learning success.Modern Language Journal 76:67–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J., et al. 1994. Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions.Journal of Educational Psychology 86:6–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. 1983.Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. 1988. Grammaticality judgments and reading skill in grade 2.Annals of Dyslexia 38: 73–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. 1991. How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. InPhonological Processes in Literacy, eds. S. Brady and D. Shankweiler. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1986. Learning disabilities and foreign language difficulties: Deficit in listening skills?”Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning Disabilities International 2:305–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1987. The foreign language requirement.Learning Disabilities Focus 2:116–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1993. Foreign language learning disabilities: Issues, research and teaching implications. InSuccess for College Students with Learning Disabilities, eds. S. Vogel and P. Adelman. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1994. Phonological processing and foreign language learning problems: Parallels between at-risk and not-at-risk foreign language learners. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Orton Dyslexia Society, November, Los Angeles, CA.

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1995a. Effects of direct instruction in phonology on the native language skills and foreign language aptitude of at-risk foreign language learners.Journal of Learning Disabilities 28:107–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., and Sparks, R. 1995b. Foreign language anxiety among high school women. (Manuscript submitted for publication).

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Anderson, R., Javorsky, J., Skinner, S. and Patton, J. 1994. Differences in language performance among high, average, and low anxious college foreign language learners.Modern Language Journal 78:41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Javorsky, J., and Patton, J. 1992. Factors relating to learning a foreign language among high- and low-risk students and students with learning disabilities.Applied Language Learning 3:37–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J., and Bishop-Marbury, A. 1991. Identifying native language difficulties among foreign language learners in college: A ‘foreign’ language learning disability?Journal of Learning Disabilities 24:530–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. 1985.Social Psychology and Second Language Learning: The Role of Attitudes and Motivation. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R., and Lambert, W. 1972.Attitude and Motivation in Seond Language Learning. Rowbery, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gass, S., and Selinker, L. 1994.Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., and Baddeley, A. 1993.Working Memory and Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guiora, A., Brannon, R. and Dull, C. 1972. Empathy and second language learning.Language Learning 22:111–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, D. 1987. Prediction of foreign language skills: A canonical and part canonical correlation study.Contemporary Educational Psychology 12:119–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horwitz, E., Horwitz, M., and Cope, J. 1986. Foreign language classroom anxiety.Modern Language Journal 70:125–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horwitz, E., and Young, D. (eds.). 1991.Language Anxiety: From Theory and Research to Classroom Implications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humes-Bartlo, M. 1989. Variation in children’s ability to learn second languages. InBilingualism Across the Lifespan, eds. K. Hyltenstam and L. Obler. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Javorsky, J., Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1992. Perceptions of college students with and without specific learning disabilities about foreign language courses.Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice 7:31–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavale, K., and Forness, S. 1990. Substance over style: A response to Dunn’s animadversions.Exceptional Children 3:357–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. 1992. The effects of lower-level processing skills on foreign language reading performance: Implications for instruction.Modern Language Journal 76:502–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. 1982.Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leutenegger, R., and Mueller, T. 1964. Auditory factors and the acquisition of French language mastery.Modern Language Journal 48:141–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leutenegger, R., Mueller, T., and Wershow, I. 1965. Auditory factors in foreign language acquisition.Modern Language Journal 49:22–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. 1985. Should so called modality preferences determine the nature of instruction for children with reading disabilities? InDyslexia, eds. F. Duffy and N. Geschwind. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I., Shankweiler, D. and Liberman, A. 1989. The alphabetic principle and learning to read. InPhonology and Reading Disability, eds. D. Shankweiler and I. Liberman. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, M. 1990. Maturational constraints on language development.Studies in Second Language Acquisition 12:251–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, P., and Gardner, R. 1991. Language anxiety: Its relationship to other anxieties and to processing in native and second languages.Language Learning 41:513–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, P., and Gardner, R. 1994. The subtle effects of anxiety on language processing in the second language.Language Learning 44:283–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V., Cowin, E., and Schoenheimer, J. 1990. Phonological processing, language comprehension, and reading ability. InCognitive and Behavioral Characteristics of Children with Learning Disabilities, ed. J. Torgesen. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V., Shankweiler, D., and Smith, S. 1984. The association between comprehension of spontaneous sentences and early reading ability: The role of phonetic representation.Journal of Child Language 11:627–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oxford, R. 1990a. Styles, strategies, and aptitude: Connections for language learning. InLanguage Aptitude Reconsidered, eds. T. Parry and C. Stansfield. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oxford R. 1990b. Language learning strategies and beyond: A look at strategies in the context of styles. InShifting the Instructional Focus to the Learner, ed. S. Magnan. Middlebury, VT: Northeast Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimsleur, P. 1966a.Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery and Manual. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimsleur, P. 1966b. Testing foreign language learning. InTrends in Language Teaching, ed. A. Valdman. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimsleur, P. 1968. Language aptitude testing. InLanguage Testing Symposium: A Linguistic Approach, ed. A. Davies. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pimsleur, P., Sundland, D., and McIntyre, R. 1964. Underachievement in foreign language learning.International Review of Applied Linguistics 2:113–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seashore, C. 1938.The Psychology of Music. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Service, E. 1992. Phonology, working memory, and foreign language learning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 45A:21–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., and Crain S. 1986. Language mechanisms and reading disorder: A modular approach.Cognition 24:139–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skehan, P. 1986. The role of foreign language aptitude in a model of school learning.Language Testing 3:188–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skehan, P. 1989.Individual Differences in Second Language Learning. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skehan, P. 1991. Individual differences in second language learning.Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13:275–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S., Mann, V., and Shankweiler, D. 1986. Spoken sentence comprehension by good and poor readers: A study with the Token Test.Cortex 22:627–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1991. Foreign language learning difficulties: Affective or native language aptitude differences?Modern Language Journal 75:3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L 1993a. Searching for the cognitive locus of foreign language learning difficulties: Linking first and second language learning.Modern Language Journal 77:289–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1993b. The impact of native language learning problems on foreign language learning: Case study illustrations of the Linguistic Coding Deficit Hypothesis.Modern Language Journal 77:58–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1993c. The effects of a multisensory structured language approach on the native and foreign language aptitude of at-risk foreign language learners: A follow-up and replication study.Annals of Dyslexia 43:194–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1993d. Identifying and instructing at-risk foreign language learners in college. InThe Dynamics of Language Program Direction, ed. d. Benseler. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1994.Raw Data.

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1995a. A strong inference approach to causal factors in foreign language learning: A response to MacIntyre.Modern Language Journal. 79:235–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. 1995b. Parent perceptions in the screening for performance in foreign language courses.Foreign Language Annals. 28:371–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. in press. Teachers’ perceptions of students’ foreign language academic skills and affective characteristics.Journal of Educational Research.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Artzer, M. 1994. Connections between native language skill and foreign language proficiency. Paper presented at the American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Languages, Atlanta, November, Atlanta, GA.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Artzer, M., and Patton, J. in press-a. Foreign language proficiency of at-risk and not-at-risk learners over two years of foreign language instruction: A follow-up study.Journal of Learning Disabilities.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Fluharty, K., and Little, S. in press-b. An exploratory study on the effects of Latin on the native language skills and foreign language aptitude of students with and without learning disabilities.Classical Journal.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Javorsky, J. 1992. Diagnosing and accommodating the foreign language learning difficulties of college students with learning disabilities.Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice 7:150–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Javorsky, J. 1993. Perceptions of low and high risk students and students with learning disabilities about high school foreign language courses.Foreign Language Annals 26:491–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Javorsky, J. in press. I know one when I see one (or, I know one because I am one): A response to Mabbott.Foreign Language Annals.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J., and Patton, J. 1992a. Identifying native language deficits in high and low risk foreign language learners in high school.Foreign Language Annals 25:403–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J., and Patton, J. 1992b. Test comparisons among students identified as high-risk, low-risk, and learning disabled in high school foreign language courses.Modern Language Journal 76:142–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Kenneweg, S., and Miller, K. 1991. Use of an Orton-Gillingham approach to teach a foreign language to dyslexic/learning disabled students: Explicit teaching of phonology in a second language.Annals of Dyslexia 41:96–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Patton, J. in press. Prediction of performance in first-year foreign language courses: Connections between native and foreign language learning.Journal of Educational Psychology.

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., and Pohlman, J. 1989. Linguistic coding deficits in foreign language learners.Annals of Dyslexia 39:179–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R., Ganschow, L., Pohlman, J., Artzer, M., and Skinner, S. 1992. The effects of a multisensory, structured language approach on the native and foreign language aptitude skills of high-risk foreign language learners.Annals of Dyslexia 42:25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1986a. Explaining the variance in reading ability in terms of psychological processes: What have we learned?Annals of Dyslexia 36:67–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1986b. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.Reading Research Quarterly 21:360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1986c. Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. InPsychological and Educational Perspectives on Learning Disabilities, eds. J. Torgesen and B. Wong. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 1988. The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability.Annals of Dyslexia 38:154–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, L. 1991. Discrepancy definitions of reading disability: Has intelligence led us astray?Reading Research Quarterly 26:7–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K., and Siegel, L. 1994. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable difference model.Journal of Educational Psychology 86:24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedemann, J. 1989. Measures of cognitive styles: A critical review.Educational Psychologist 24:261–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F., and Scanlon, D. 1986. Linguistic coding and metalinguistic awareness: Their relationship to verbal and code acquisition in poor and normal readers. InMetalinguistic Awareness and Beginning Literacy, eds. D. Yaden and S. Templeton. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R., and Torgesen, J. 1987. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.Psychological Bulletin 101:192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig E, and Semel, E. 1980.Language Assessment and Intervention for the Learning Disabled. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sparks, R.L. Examining the linguistic coding differences hypothesis to explain individual differences in foreign language learning. Annals of Dyslexia 45, 187–214 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648218

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648218

Keywords

Navigation