Abstract
Although problem solving is regarded by most educators as among the most important learning outcomes, few instructional design prescriptions are available for designing problem-solving instruction and engaging learners. This paper distinguishes between well-structured problems and ill-structured problems. Well-structured problems are constrained problems with convergent solutions that engage the application of a limited number of rules and principles within well-defined parameters. Ill-structured problems possess multiple solutions, solution paths, fewer parameters which are less manipulable, and contain uncertainty about which concepts, rules, and principles are necessary for the solution or how they are organized and which solution is best. For both types of problems, this paper presents models for how learners solve them and models for designing instruction to support problem-solving skill development. The model for solving well-structured problems is based on information processing theories of learning, while the model for solving ill-structured problems relies on an emerging theory of ill-structured problem solving and on constructivist and situated cognition approaches to learning.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning.References
Anderson, J.R., Farrell, R. & Sauers, R. (1984). Learning to program in LISP.Cognitive Science, 8, 87–129.
Arlin, P.K. (1989). The problem of the problem. In J.D. Sinnott (Ed.),Everyday problem solving: Theory and applications (pp. 229–237). New York: Praeger.
Bloom, B.S., Englehart, M.D., Furst, E.J., Hill, W.H., & Krathwohl, D.R. (1956).Taxonomy of educational objectives: The classification of educational goals: Handbook 1; The cognitive domain. New York: Longman.
Bodker, S. (1991). Activity theory as a challenge to systems design. In H.E. Nissen, H.K. Klein, & R. Hirschheim (Eds.),Information systems research: Contemporary approaches and emergent traditions. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bransford, J. (1994). Who ya gonna call? Thoughts about teaching problem solving. In P. Hallinger, K. Lithwood, & J. Murphy (Eds.),Cognitive perspectives on educational leadership. New York: Teacher's College Press.
Bransford, J., & Stein, B.S. (1984).The IDEAL problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity.New York:W.H. Freeman.
Charney, D., Reder, L., & Kusbit, G.W. (1990). Goal setting and procedure selection in acquiring computer skills: A comparison of tutorials, problem solving, and learner exploration.Cognition & Instruction, 7(4) 323–342.
Chi, M.T.H., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
Chi, M.T.H. & Glaser, R. (1985). Problem solving ability. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.),Human abilities: An information processing approach. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Chi, M.T.H. & Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.),Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (pp. 7–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Churchman, C.W. (1971).The design of inquiring systems: Basic concepts of systems and organizations. New York: Basic Books.
Cognition Technology Group at Vanderbilt (1992). Technology and the design of generative learning environments. In T.M. Duffy & D.H. Jonassen (Eds.),Constructivism and the technology of instruction: A conversation (pp. 77–89). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation of mathematical problem-solving transfer.Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 347–362.
Covington, M.C., Crutchfield, R.S., Daves, L.B., & Olton, R.M. (1974).The productive thinking program: A course in learning to think. Columbus, Ohio: Charles Merrill.
de Jong, T., & Ferguson-Hessler, M.G.M. (1986). Cognitive structures of good an poor novice problem solvers in physics.Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 279–288.
deBono, E. (1974).CoRT thinking materials. London: Direct Educational Services.
Dunkle, M.E., Schraw, G., & Bendixen, L.D. (1995, April).Cognitive processes in well-defined and ill-defined problem solving. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.
Ernst, G.W., & Newell, A. (1969).GPS: A case study in generality and problem solving. New York: Academic Press.
Gagné, R.M. (1966).The conditions of learning (1st ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Gagné, R.M. (1977).The conditions of learning (3rd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Gagné, R.M. (1985).The conditions of learning (4th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Gagné, R.M., Briggs, L.J., & Wager, W.W. (1992).Principles of instructional design (4th ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace, & Jovanovich.
Gick, M.L. (1986). Problem-solving strategies.Educational Psychologist, 21, 99–120.
Gick, M.L., & Holyoak, K.J. (1980). Analogical problem solving.Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355.
Gordon, S.E., & Gill, R.T.. (1989). The formation and use of knowledge structures in problem solving domains. Tech. Report AFOSR-88-0063. Washington, DC: Bolling AFB.
Greeno, J. (1978). Natures of problem-solving abilities. In W. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (pp. 239–270). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hayes, J.R. (1981)The compleat problem solver. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press.
Jacobson, M.J. (1990).Knowledge acquisition, cognitive flexibility, and the instructional applications of hypertext: A comparison of contrasting designs for computer-enhanced learning environments. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois).
Jonassen, D.H. (1996). Scaffolding diagnostic reasoning in case-based learning environments.Journal of Computing in Higher Education.
Jonassen, D.H., Ambruso, D.R., & Olesen, J. (1992). Designing a hypertext on transfusion medicine using cognitive flexibility theory.Journal of Educational Hypermedia and Multimedia, 1(3), 309–322.
Jonassen, D.H., Beissner, K., & Yacci, M.A. (1993).Structural knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jonassen, D.H., Doricott, D., & Engels, R. (1995).Comparing the effectiveness of worked examples and trial-and-error learning methods in an engineering microworld. American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.
Jonassen, D.H., Mann, E., & Ambruso, D.J. (1996). Causal modeling for structuring case-based learning environments.Intelligent Tutoring Media, 6(3/4), 103–112.
Jonassen, D.H., & Tessmer, M. (1996). An outcomes-based taxonomy for the design, evaluation, and research of instructional systems.Training Research Journal.
Kosonen, P., & Winne, P.H. (1995). Effects of teaching statistical laws on reasoning about everyday problems.Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 33–46.
Kitchner, K.S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition, and epistemic cognition: A three-level model of cognitive processing.Human Development, 26, 222–232.
Kitchner, K.S., & King, P.M. (1981). Reflective judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89–116.
Kluwe, R.H., & Friedrichsen, G. (1985), Mechanisms of control and regulation and problem solving. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.),Action control (pp. 203–217). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Kuutti, K. (1996). Activity theory as a potential framework for human-computer interaction research. In B.A Nardi (Ed.),Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lave, J. (1988).Cognition in practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
LeBlanc, S.E., & Fogler, H.S. (1995).Strategies for creative problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Leont'ev, A.N. (1978).Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mayer, R.E. (1989). Models for understanding.Review of Educational Research, 59(1), 43–64.
McCombs, B.L. (1986). The instructional systems development model: A review of those factors critical to its implementation.Educational Communications and Technology Journal, 34, 67–81.
Meacham, J.A., & Emont, N.C. (1989). The interpersonal basis of everyday problem solving. In J.D. Sinnott (Ed.),Everyday problem solving: Theory and applications (pp. 7–23). New York: Praeger.
Merrill, M.D. (1983). Component display theory. In C.M. Reigeluth (Ed.),Instructional design theories and models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nardi, B.A. (1996).Context and consciousness: Activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Newell, A. (1980). Reasoning, problem solving and decision processes: The problem space as a fundamental category. In R.S. Nickerson (Ed.),Attention and performance: Proceedings of the International Symposium on Attention and Performance, VIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Newell, A. & Simon, H. (1972).Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Polson. P., & Jeffries, R. (1985). Instruction in problem solving skills: An analysis of four approaches. In J.W. Segal, S. F. Chipman, & R. Glaser (Eds.),Thinking and learning skills (Vol. 1, pp. 417–455). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Polya, M. (1957).How to solve it (2nd Ed.). New York: Doubleday.
Reed, S. K. (1992).Cognition: Theory and applications. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Reitman, W. (1965).Cognition and thought. New York: Wiley.
Robertson, W.C. (1990). Detection of cognitive structure with protocol data: Predicting performance on physics transfer problems.Cognitive Science, 14, 253–280.
Rubinstein, M. (1975).Patterns of problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Scandura, J.M. (1964). An analysis of exposition and discovery modes of problem solving instruction.Journal of Experimental Education, 33(2), 149–159.
Schank, R., & Cleary, C. (1995).Engines for education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schön, D.A. (1990). The design process. In V.A. Howard (Ed.),Varieties of thinking: Essays from Harvard's philosophy of education center (pp. 110–141). New York: Routledge.
Simon, H.A. (1976). Identifying basic abilities underlying intelligent performance on complex tasks. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.),The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Simon, D.P. (1978). Information processing theory of human problem solving. In D. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Simon, D.P., & Simon, H.A. (1978). Individual differences in solving physics problems. In R. Siegler (Ed.),Children's thinking: What develops. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sinnott, J.D. (1989). A model for solution of ill-structured problems: Implications for everyday and abstract problem solving. In J.D. Sinnott (Ed.),Everyday problem solving: Theory and applications (pp. 72–99). New York: Praeger.
Smith, P.L., & Ragan, T.J. (1993).Instructional design. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Spiro, R.J., Feltovich, P.J., Jacobson, M.J., & Coulson, R.L. (1992). Cognitive flexibility, constructivism, and hypertext: Random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. In T.M. Duffy & D.H. Jonassen (Eds.),Constructivism and the technology of instruction: A conversation (pp. 57–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spiro, R.J., Vispoel, W., Schmitz, J., Samarapungavan, A., & Boerger, A. (1987). Knowledge acquisition for application: Cognitive flexibility and transfer in complex content domains. In B.C. Britton (Ed.),Executive control processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spiro, R.J., Coulson, R.L., Feltovich, P.J., & Anderson, D.K. (1988).Cognitive flexibility theory: Advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. Tech Report No. 441. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.
Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. (1985) The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra.Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89.
Tarmizi, R., & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving.Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 424–436.
Voss, J.F. (1988). Learning and transfer in subject-matter learning: A problem solving model.International Journal of Educational Research, 11, 607–622.
Voss, J.F., & Post, T.A. (1989). On the solving of ill-structured problems. In M.T.H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M.J. Farr (Eds.),The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Voss, J.F., Wolfe, C.R., Lawrence, J.A., & Engle, R.A. (1991). From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.),Complex problem solving. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ward, M., & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples.Cognition and Instruction, 7(1), 1–39.
Wickelgren, W.A., (1973).How to solve problems: Elements of a theory of problems and problem solving. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.
Wood, P.K. (1983). Inquiring systems and problem structures: Implications for cognitive development.Human Development, 26, 249–265.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
His publications include the recently completedHandbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology, reviewed in this issue.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Jonassen, D.H. Instructional design models for well-structured and III-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. ETR&D 45, 65–94 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02299613
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02299613