Skip to main content

“Visual Vivencias”: A Cultural-Historical Tool for Understanding the Lived Experiences of Young Children’s Everyday Lives

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Educational Research with Our Youngest

Abstract

The aim of this chapter is to present a theorisation of “Visual Vivencias” as a method for studying children aged 3 years and younger and, through this, contribute to a new cultural-historical understanding of early childhood research. We specifically introduce a case example that took place in a Mexican family in Monterrey, Nuevo Leon, Mexico, as part of a wider project reported elsewhere (Quiñones, 2011).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bozhovich, L. (2009). The social situation of child development. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(4), 59–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durán, J. L., & Myers, R. (2006). Análisis de la Prueba en Campo de la Escala de Desarrollo de Competencias en Niños y Niñas de 1, 3 y 6 años de edad. Informe 3 del Estudio de Campo para Pilotear Instrumentos y Metodologías de Indicadores del Desarrollo, Calidad de Programas y Contextos. Preparado para UNICEF y Fundación General Electric. Hacia una Cultura Democrática, A.C. Documento no publicado. Retrieved from http://www.acude.org.mx/bienestar-proyecto2.html

  • Ferholt, B. (2009). The development of cognition, emotion, imagination and creativity as made visible through adult-child joint play: Perezhivanie through playworlds. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of San California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferholt, B. (2010). A synthetic–analytic method for the study of perezhivanie: Vygotsky’s literary analysis applied to playworlds. In M. C. Connery, V. P. John-Steiner, & A. Marjanovic-Shane (Eds.), Vygotsky and creativity. A cultural-historical approach to play, meaning making and the arts (pp. 163–179). New York, NY: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (2008a). The relations between cultural-historical theory, methodology and digital video methods. In M. Hedegaard & M. Fleer (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 104–117). London, England: Open University Press,

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (2008b). Interpreting research protocols. In M. Hedegaard & M. Fleer (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 65–87). London, England: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (2008c). Interpreting research protocols: The child’s perspective. In M. Hedegaard & M. Fleer (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 88–103). London, England: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. (2000). Investigación cualitativa en psicología. Rumbos y desafíos. Mexico: Thomson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. (2002). L. S. Vygotsky and the question of personality in the cultural approach. In D. Robbins & A. Stetsenko (Eds.), Voices within Vygotsky’s non-classical psychology: Past, present, future (pp. 129–142). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. (2009a). Historical relevance of Vygotsky’s work: Its significance for a new approach to the problem of subjectivity in psychology. Outlines, 1, 59–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. (2009b). La significación de Vygotski para la consideración de lo afectivo en la educación: Las bases para la cuestión de la subjetividad [Vygotsky’s significance for consideration of affective processes in education: A new basis for the topic of subjectivity]. Actualidades Investigativas en Educacion, 9, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. (2011). Advancing on the concept of sense: Subjective sense and subjective configurations in human development. In M. Hedegaard, A. Edward, & M. Fleer (Eds.), Motives, emotions and values in children’s and youth’s development: Advances from a cultural historical approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (2008). A cultural-historical theory of children’s development. In M. Hedegaard & M. Fleer (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 10–29). London, England: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M., & Fleer, M. (2008). Studying children: A cultural-historical approach. London, England: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • John-Steiner, V. P., Connery, M. C., & Marjanovic-Shane, A. (2010). Dancing with the muses: A cultural-historical approach to play, meaning making and creativity. In M. C. Connery, V. P. John-Steiner, & A. Marjanovic-Shane (Eds.), Vygotsky and creativity: A cultural–historical approach to play, meaning making and the arts (pp. 3–15). New York, NY: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpov, Y. (2005). The neo-Vygotskian approach to child development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravtsova, G. G., & Kravtsov, E. E. (2009). Cultural-historical psychology in the practice of education. In M. Fleer, M. Hedegaard, & J. Tudge (Eds.), Childhood studies and the impact of globalization: Policies and practices at global and local levels (pp. 202–212). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, A., Correa-Chavez, M., Rogoff, B., & Gutierrez, K. (2010). Attention to instruction directed to another by US Mexican-heritage children of varying cultural backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 46(3), 593–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mejia-Arauz, R., Rogoff, B., & Paradise, R. (2005). Cultural variation in children’s observation during a demonstration. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(4), 282–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, R., & Durán, J. L. (2006). Análisis de la Prueba en Campo del Instrumento de Evaluación de los Contextos Hogar Familiar y Comunitario. Informe 4 del Estudio de Campo para Pilotear Instrumentos y Metodologías de Indicadores del Desarrollo, Calidad de Programas y Contextos. Retrieved from: http://www.acude.org.mx/bienestar-proyecto2.html

  • Myers, R., Durán, J. L., Guerreo, V., González, V., & Meléndez, C. (2005). Descripción del Trabajo en Campo. Informe 1 del Estudio de Campo para Pilotear Instrumentos y Metodologías de Indicadores del Desarrollo, Calidad de Programas y Contextos. Preparado para UNICEF y Fundación General Electric. Hacia una Cultura Democrática, A.C. Documento no publicado. Retrieved from http://www.acude.org.mx/biblioteca_bienestar.html

  • Quiñones, G. (2011). Dialectical relationships of children’s vivencias across home, school and community in Monterrey, Nuevo Leon, Mexico. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Monash University, Faculty of Education, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SEP Secretaria de Educación Pública [Secretary of Public Education]. (2004a). Curso de Formación y Actualización Profesional para el Personal Docente de Educación Preescolar: Volumen I [Training and professional actualization for preschool teachers: Volume I]. México: SEP Secretaria de Educación Pública.

    Google Scholar 

  • SEP Secretaria de Educación Pública. (2004b). Programa de Educación Preescolar 2004 [Educational Preschool Program]. México: SEP Secretaria de Educación Pública.

    Google Scholar 

  • UNICEF Foro de las Naciones Unidas para la Infancia & SEP Secretaria de Educación Pública. (2002). Foro sobre indicadores de bienestar en la primera infancia en México. Resumen. N. Naime, M. E. Ocampo, E. V, Butze, A. Nava, R. Myers, M. Kagoshima, M. Cruz, V. Guerrero, B. García. Fondo de las Naciones Unidas para la Infancia (UNICEF). Retrieved from: http://www.sep.gob.mx/work/appsite/indicadore/resumen.pdf

  • Veresov, N. (2006). Leading activity in development psychology: Concept and principle. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 44(5), 7–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1: Thinking and speech. New York, NY: Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1993). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky: Vol. 2: The fundamentals of defectology. New York, NY: Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of environment. In R. Van Der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 338–354). Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 5. Child psychology. New York, NY: Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the coordinator, teacher and members of the school for the warm welcome given to the researcher. We also wish to acknowledge the trust given to Gloria to research the everyday life of Cesar and his family. All Spanish to English translations were done by the first author and data examples given were derived from the first author’s Ph.D. study of Mexican families.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gloria Quiñones .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Commentary to Gloria Quiñones and Marilyn Fleer: “Visual Vivencias”

On my initial reading of this chapter I was struck by its clarity in relation to the research journey that unfolds about a young Mexican boy, Cesar, and his homework activity through the employment of analytical snapshots that are underpinned by carefully articulated methodological imperatives. The detailed description of the methodology that underpins this data provides the reader with a rare glimpse into what, I believe, constitutes a new and exciting operationalisation of sociocultural theory in the form of Visual Vivencias. I am in no doubt that, for this reason alone, the chapter will be inspirational for researchers grappling with meaningful ways of entering into the complex world of the very young child.

The interpretation of Russian perezhivanie in relation to Spanish vivencias is a particularly interesting and fruitful contribution to the research arena, especially when considered from a cultural-historical standpoint. In this location the authors seem to be suggesting, with Vygotsky (1994), that lived emotional experience of the individual learner is the central point of gestalt for the researcher; and that it is located in the dialectical relationship that takes place between the environment and the child’s emotional engagement with that environment. I interpret this to mean that the environment, per se, holds relevance to the child’s learning or experience only when it is imbued with meaning through interaction. This is an important claim for research with very young children, since, as the authors point out, so much research with this age group (including in a non-Western society such as Mexico) is heavily weighted by measures of competency against pre-determined measures that are located “in the environment” without consideration of emotional engagement, meaning and experience on the part of the learner.

The “wholeness approach” to research advocated by the authors in their research design represents considerable challenges when we consider affect and cognition as a dynamic system in unity. It is one that has perplexed researchers across the globe and, in early childhood research, led to the development of significant frameworks for analysis (see for example the work of Carr, 2009, and Clarke, 2007, among others). Several neo-Vygotskians have attempted to respond to the challenge drawing specifically from Vygotsky, including the foci of analysis model (Rogoff, 1995) and activity theory (Engeström, Miettinen, & Punamäki, 1999). In this chapter, the authors draw from Hedegaard’s (2008) framework—a recent addition to the field which bears close allegiance to these models; and invites the researcher to view institutional practices and individual activities as interrelated concepts. However, Quiñones and Fleer go further to suggest that a wholeness approach should also be concerned with intellect and affect, providing additional provocations to guide the researcher towards dialectic approaches in research activity. It is here they invoke perezhavanie as a unit of analysis.

If we take this idea seriously, as these authors most certainly have, the researcher must somehow search for meaning from the perspective of this child in specific contexts (that is, the life-world of this child), while recognising the prevailing influences of the wider world—a constant quest for researchers and teachers alike in claiming any interpretation of “voice”; and one that is at the very heart of this collection. With Hedegaard’s inspiration, the authors of this chapter have a means of doing so. As many of the contributors have already pointed out, this is not an easy task when the very young child does not share the same semiotic language system as the adult, in particular the researcher. It was exciting, therefore, to read of the authors’ cognisance of non-verbal forms of language in understanding the vivencias of Cesar, and the important part they play in meaning making for the very young learners. The sections which explore skills and non-verbal communication provide a particularly useful example of how interpretation might be possible using Visual Vivencia as a methodology. Given Vygotsky’s (1987) claim that infants do not have the capacity for human speech, this section provides a very useful embellishment of the way a young child engages with human speech in subtle yet complex ways.

I was consistently drawn to the authors’ interpretation of perezhivanie throughout this chapter. This is a Russian concept also employed by Voloshinov (1978), who suggests that meaning is always evaluative and therefore must have significance for the particular group in which acts take place (Liberali, 2009). Bakhtin’s additional use of the Russian term postupok (Bakhtin, 1993)—an answerable act or deed—suggests that the gestalt of this lived experience is determined not so much by the environment in relation to the individual, but by the people in that environment and the strategic orientation of the individual toward them in the language acts they present (Marjanovic-Shane, 2011). Given that vivencias is also proposed as an extension of the term, and that it seems to take into particular account the strong influence of family and society, I wonder if further attention to these orientations would be valuable in studies of very young children across cultures. In making this suggestion it occurs to me that there is much scope for enriched interpretations of key theoretical constructs when language is shared across cultures and societies as a means of understanding complex ideas. Such an approach, however, requires reflexive positioning on the part of researchers if their ideas are to be taken seriously across cultural and conceptual borders, and offers less universal certainty than many researchers are accustomed to. The dialogues that take place around these concepts help us to clarify our meaning as well as those of others, and to recognise the interpretative (and cultural) nature of research in this domain.

In any case the authors’ solution is sought in an approach they carefully describe as a “dialectical-interactive”. This method, developed by Hedegaard and Fleer (2008), allows the researcher to study everyday activity, in this case homework, in complex ways that fulfil their promise of expanding on the notion of competencia to take account of the dialectics at play. Like others in this book, their route is through video, and the employment of stimulated dialogue using that video. Here the emphasis seems to shift from “child vivencias” to those of “mother and child vivencias” (although this is not explicitly expounded) as a unit whose members are assumed to share the same emotional experience or speak for one another—a notion I ponder since I concur with Linell (2010) that the infant needs to be treated as a research subject in their own right (however hard that is). Having said that, it is important to note that the examples provided in this chapter are part of a larger study involving home and school dialectics that are not discussed here, so the emphasis was less on the home as a sole institution than the home environment as an important part of the wider educational experience. Such is the value of research of this nature, since the authors are able to recognise a far more complex picture by investigating such systems.

On a more pragmatic level, it was surprising to learn that Mexican approaches to very young children are so heavily influenced by discrete competencies with such emphasis on skills and physical development. It caused me to reflect on the significant influence and power welded by educational institutions on what families see as important learning—an aspect of discussion that this research explores in the wider study. However, I would suggest that the researchers’ choice to focus on homework as a leading activity for investigation lent itself more to this scholastic emphasis than other activities may have. It would be interesting to see this research repeated, in the home, with other cultural activities in mind, since it is likely that these hold significant emotional value to the child (indeed, it could be argued that they would hold more emotional value to Cesar). As one of very few sociocultural studies that has been conducted in this locale, this chapter provides a beacon of hope in research that is conducted in and between home and other locales; since it is clear that any cultural-historical interpretation cannot ignore this significant institution in the life and learning of a young child. In Cesar’s case, the chosen vivencia raises important, but unexplored, questions concerning the emotional value of homework as an educational activity for the very young child despite its value to his family.

To conclude, not only does this chapter provide important incentives for researchers to consider the wider contexts of the child’s life-world, but it also sets the scene for future research that investigates the vivencias of young children in an increasingly diasporic world (I make this claim for both the researcher and the researched). The inspirational examples outlined here, underpinned by a unique methodological framework, provide a potential route to the emotional lived experience of the infant and toddler as a means of bringing cognition/competency and emotion into dialectic interplay. Moreover, the notion of visual vivencia provides researchers with inspiration to go beyond current frameworks to develop methodologically sound approaches that best suit their purpose. Theorising of how best to understand and appreciate young children through such exploration is a route we must inevitably take if we are serious about deepening our understandings of our youngest children, ourselves, and the societies in which we live.

References

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1993). Toward a philosophy of the act (V. Liapunov & M. Holquist, Eds.; 1st ed.). Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

  • Carr, M. (2009). Kei tua o te pae: Assessing learning that reaches beyond the self and beyond the horizon, Assessment matters, 1, 20–46.

  • Clarke, A. (2007). A hundred ways of listening: Gathering children’s perspectives of their early childhood environment. Young Children, 62(3), 76–81.

  • Engeström, Y., Miettinen, R., & Punamäki, R.-L. (Eds.). (1999). Activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 19–38). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Hedegaard, M. (2008). A cultural-historical theory of children’s development. In M. Hedegaard & M. Fleer (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 10–29). London, England: Open University Press.

  • Hedegaard, M., & Fleer, M. (2008). Studying children: A cultural-historical approach. London, England: Open University Press.

  • Liberali, F. C., (2009). Creative chain in the process of becoming a totality: A cadeia criativa no processo de tornar-se totalidade. Bakhtinia, 1(2), 100–124.

  • Linell, P. (2010). With respect to Bakhtin: Some trends in contemporary dialogical theories. In K. Junefelt & P. Nordin (Eds.), Proceedings from the Second International Interdisciplinary Conference on Perspectives and Limits of Dialogism in Mikhail Bakhtin (pp. 18–32). Stockholm, Sweden: Stockholm University.

  • Marjanovic-Shane, A. (2011). You are Nobody!—The architectonics of play. In E. J. White & M. Peters (Eds.), Global studies in education: Bakhtinian pedagogy; opportunities and challenges for research, policy and practice in education across the globe. New York: Peter Lang.

  • Rogoff, B. (1995). Observing sociocultural activity on three planes: Participatory appropriation, guided participation, and apprenticeship. In J. Wertsch, P. del Rio, & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 139–164). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Voloshinov, V. N. (1978). Marxism and the philosophy of language (L. Matejka & I. R. Titunik, Trans.; Vol. 1). New York: Seminar Press.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol. 4: The history and development of higher mental functions. New York: Plenum Publishers.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of environment. In R. Van Der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 338–354). Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers.

Glossary

Dialectics:

We refer to dialectics when there is a dynamical relationship between the individual and the social environment consisting of social relationships and the surrounding place. This concept usually reflects opposites and counterparts; we here refer to the unity and dynamics of the relationship of the individual and environment.

Perezhivanie :

Vygotsky (1994) referred to perezhivanie as emotional living experience. This concept reflects the methodological problem Vygotsky refers in his writing about environment.

Visual Vivencias:

is a methodological tool that uses photographs of moments in time of the research. The program used was “Windows Live Movie Maker”, which captures in a photograph a snapshot of the whole activity of the child. This research aims to capture these moments visually for multiple interpretations and not just of the researchers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Quiñones, G., Fleer, M. (2011). “Visual Vivencias”: A Cultural-Historical Tool for Understanding the Lived Experiences of Young Children’s Everyday Lives. In: Johansson, E., White, E. (eds) Educational Research with Our Youngest. International perspectives on early childhood education and development, vol 5. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2394-8_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics