Skip to main content

Cognition and Second Language Experience: How Are Executive Function and Second Language Acquisition Related?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 928 Accesses

Part of the book series: Second Language Learning and Teaching ((SLLT))

Abstract

Bilingual children’s better performance on cognitive tasks has been explained by greater proficiency in executive function (EF) compared with monolingual peers. This is postulated to stem from quality and complexity in their linguistic environment. Many international studies of executive function adopt leading indicators such as academic performance, overall well-being and happiness. This chapter takes a broader view on bilingualism, including child experience of instructed second language (L2) acquisition and research attempts to map relationships between this experience and EF. The focus is on investigations of causality and studies of the bidirectional influence between EF and L2, suggesting that individual childhood differences improve them as L2 learners and that early L2 experience, in turn, commands a lasting influence on EF. The controversy of the claimed bilingual cognitive advantage is also discussed, and methodological issues are raised. A recent call to re-examine EF to include a broader range of the skills relied upon by children to achieve specific goals is briefly introduced with implications for future studies of the EF/L2 relationship.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    During the Simon task, participants are asked to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible to a stimulus, e.g.,: a red or green square, by pressing a key or a button. In congruent trials a response is required with the same laterality as the stimulus presentation and in incongruent trials the response and the stimulus are presented at opposite sides. The Simon Effect is the proportion of incongruent to congruent trials with lower scores indicating a lower switching cost and it is therefore used as an indicator for individual, simple processing speed differences.

  2. 2.

    Goriot et al (2018) showed no relationship between L1/L2 balance and working memory (WM)—contrary to Bloom et al. (2014) for Dutch-Turkish bilinguals—and there is some speculation that if such a relationship between working memory does exist it might exist in more advanced L2 learners. Bilingual advantage in working memory (WM) was not shown by Barac et al. (2014).

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Brambati, S. M., Annoni, J. M., Moro, A,. Cappa, S. E., & Perani, D. (2007). The nueral cost of the auditory perception of language switches: An event-related functinal magnetic resonance study in bilinguals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 13761–13769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics,20, 242–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research,80, 207–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Elliott, J. (2010). Examining the link between working memory behaviour and academic attainment in children with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,52(7), 632–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of Executive Function (EF) during childhoold. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ardila, A. (1999). A neuropsychological approach to intelligence. Neuropsychology Review, 9(3), 117–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkins, P. W. B., & Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics,19(4), 537–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS ONE,11(4), e0153485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive ageing? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959–963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bak, T. H., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2014). Never too late? An advantage on tests of auditory attention extends to late bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,29, 699–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvillain, C., & Grainger, J. (1987). Accessing interlexical homographs: Some limitations of a language-selective access. Journal of Memory and Language,26, 658–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences,21, 327–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1986). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development,57, 498–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development,70, 636–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2013). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition,122, 67–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development,11, 485–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freeedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and congnitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859–873.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierman, K. L., Torres, M. M., Domitrovich, C. E., Welsh, J. A., & Gest, S. D. (2009). Behavioral and cognitive readiness for school: Cross-domain associations for children attending Head Start. Social Development, 18, 305–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507x2008.00490.x.

  • Blair, C. (2002). School readiness. American Psychologist,57, 111–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., Granger, D., & Razza, R. P. (2005). Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development,76, 554–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blom, E., Kuntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,128C, 105–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Paralel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 547–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science,11, 282–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoffels, I. K., Firk, C., & Schiller, N. O. (2007, February 9). Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study. Brain Research, 192–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.137. epub.

  • Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., & Olson, R. K. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,141(3), 470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., Hernandez, A., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition,113, 135–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., Hernandez, M., & Sebastiàn-Gallès, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition,106, 59–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language,50, 491–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review,26(2), 197–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., & Alloway, T. (2009). Development of working memory in childhood. In M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of memory in infancy and childhood (pp. 303–342). Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., & Morey, C. C. (2007). How can dual-task working memory retention limits be investigated? Psychological Science, 18(8), 686–688. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01960.x.

  • Cox, S., Bak, T. H., Allerhand, M., Redmond, P., Starr, J. M., Deary, I. J., & MacPherson, S. E. (2016). Bilingualism, social cognition and executive functions: A tale of chickens and eggs. Nueropsychologia,91, 299–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006.

  • Dawson, P., & Guare, R. (2004). Executive skills in children and adolescents: A practical guide to assessment and intervention. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science,26, 99–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4–12 years old. Science,333, 959–964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2015). The bilingual advantage: Acta est fabula. Cortex, 73, 371–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duñabeitia, J. A., Dimitropoulou, M., Morris, J., & Diependaele, K. (2013). The role of form in morphological priming: Evidence from bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes,28(7), 967–987. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.713972.

  • Duncan, G. J., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Kalil, A. (2010). Early childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior, and health. Child Development,81, 306–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309.

  • Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., Malmud, E. K., & Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research,1110, 166–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez, M., Tartar, J. L., Padron, D., & Acosta, J. (2013). Neurophysiological marker of inhibition distinguishes language groups on a non-linguistic executive function test. Brain and Cognition,83, 330–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 6, 5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-5.

  • Galambos, S. J., & Hakuta, K. (1988). Subject-specific and task-specific characteristics of metalinguistic awareness in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics,9, 141–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Norris, D. (2019). Working memory training involves learning new skills. Journal of Memory and Language,105, 19–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Guasch, N. V., & Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from pre-schoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011.

  • Goetz, P. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goriot, C., Broersma, M., McQueen, J. M., Unsworth, S., & van Hout, R. (2018). Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,173, 168–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Gallop, R., Romer, D., Brodsky, N. L., Hurt, H., & Farah, M. J. (2014). Mapping the trajectory of socioeconomic disparity in working memory: Parental and neighborhood factors. Child Development, 85(4), 1433–1445. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12242H.

  • Hernandez, M., Martin, C. D., Barcelo, F., & Costa, A. (2013). Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language, 69(3), 257–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoshino, N., & Thierry, G. (2011). Language selection in bilingual word production: Electrophysiological evidence for cross-language competition. Brain Research,1371, 100–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.053.

  • Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,16(2), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in ‘hard-to-manage’ preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 981–994. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00401.

  • Hussey, E., & Novick, J. (2012). The benefits of executive control training and the implications for language processing. Frontiers in Psychology,3(158), 158. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00158.

  • Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement a review. Review of Educational Research,85(4), 512–552. https://doi.org/0034654314561338.

  • Jones, S. M., Bailey, R., Barnes, S. P., & Partee, A. (2016). Executive function mapping project: Untangling the terms and skills related to executive function and self-regulation in early childhood: Executive summary. OPRE Report #2016-88, United States, Administration for Children and Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaczan, R., & Sobolewski, A. (2015). Wyniki testu i ich interpretacja. In R. Kaczan, P. Rycielski, K. Rzeńca, & K. Sijko (Eds.), Metody diagnozy na pierwszym etapie edukacyjnym. Narzędzia Diagnostyczne Instytutu Badań Edukacyjnych (pp. 245–260). Warszawa: Instytut Badań Edukacyjnych.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28, 233–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2014). Executive function predicts artificial language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 237–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G. L., & Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). The unity and diversity of executive functions: A systematic review and reanalysis of latent variable studies. Psychological Bulletin,144, 1147–1185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kibby, M. Y., Lee, S. E., & Dyer, S. M. (2014). Reading performance is predicted by more than phonological processing. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 960. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 356–369. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604788.

  • Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., & Taler, V. (2014). Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science,106, 6556–6560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., Bobb, C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica,128(3), 416–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Tokowicz, N. (2001). The development of conceptual representation for words in a second language. In J. L. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 49–71). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linck, J. A., Hughest, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., & Doughty, C. J. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63, 530–566. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12011.

  • Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing access to the native language while immersed in second language. Psychological Science, 20, 1507–1515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linck, J. A., & Weiss, D. J. (2015). Can working memory and inhibitory control predict second languae learning in the classroom? SAGE Open, 5(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015607352.

  • Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2009). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics,24(2), 173–193. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716403000092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marian, V., Spivey, M., & Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language,86, 70–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. D., Dering, B., Thomas, E. M., & Thierry, G. (2009). Brain potentials reveal semantic priming in both the ‘active’ and the ‘non-attended’ language of early bilinguals. Neuroimage,47, 326–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, K. I., & Ellis, N. C. (2012). The role of phonological short-term memory and working memory in l2 grammar and vocabulary learning. StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 34, 379–413. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263112000125.

  • Meschyan, G., & Hernandez, A. E. (2006). Impact of language proficiency and orthographic transparency on bilingual word reading: An fMRI investigation. NeuroImage,29(4), 1135–1140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 224–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.001.

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology,41, 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, S., Wodniecka, Z., Tays, W., Alain, C., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Inhibitory control in bilinguals and musicians: Event related potential (ERP) evidence for experience-specific effects. PLoS ONE,9, e94169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munoz, C. (2014). Starting age and other influential factors: Insights from learner interviews. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 4(3), 465–484. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.3.5.

  • Nisbett, R. (2010). Inteligencja. Sposoby oddziaływania na IQ [Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count]. Sopot: Smak Słowa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novick, J. M., Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Harbison, J. I., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Clearing the garden-path: Improving sentence processing through cognitive control training. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(2), 186–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.758297.

  • Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,5(3), 263–281. https://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.5.3.263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2020). Early learning and child well-being: A study of five-year-olds in England, Estonia, and the United States. OECD Publishing, Paris. https://doi.org/10.1787/3990407f-en.

  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology,66, 232–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology,26, 615–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. S., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex,69, 615–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Myuz, H. A., Anders, R. T., Bockelman, M. F., Mikulinsky, R., & Sawi, O. (2017). No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology,29, 89–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pivneva, I., Mercier, J., & Titone, D. (2014). Executive control modulates cross-language lexical activation during L2 reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 787–796. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035583.

  • Pivneva, I., Palmer, C., & Titone, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and l2 proficiency modulate bilingual language production: Evidence from spontaneous monologue and dialogue speech. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poarch, G. J. (2018). Multilingual language control and executive function: A replication study. Frontiers in Communication, 3(46). https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00046.

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). Influencing brain networks: Implications for education. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 99–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulisse, N. (2000). Slips of the tongue in first and second language production. Studia Linguistica: A Journal of General Linguistics, 54(2), 136–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,17(4), 682–691. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617711000580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526.

  • Purić, D., Vuksanović, J., & Chondrogianni, V. (2017). Cognitive advantages of immersion education after one year: Effects of amount of exposure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,159, 296–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reuhkala, M. (2001). Mathematical skills in ninth-graders: Relationship with visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Educational Psychology, 21(4), 387–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggs, N. R., Blair, C. B., & Greenberg, M. T. (2004). Concurrant and 2nd-year longitudinal relations between executive function and the behaviour of 1st and 2nd grade children. Child Neuropsychology, 9(4), 267–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Heinze, H. J., Nosselt, T. M., & Munte, T. F. (2002). Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature,415, 1026–1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubio-Fernández, P., & Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people’s beliefs: Bilinguals have an advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 211–217. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025162.

  • Schwartz, A., & Kroll, G. G. (2006). Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language,55(2), 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastian-Galles, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F. (2012). A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science,23, 994–999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sędek, G., Krejtz, I., Rydzewska, K., Kaczan, R., & Rycielski, P. (2016). Three functional aspects of working memory as strong predictors of early school achievements: The review and illustrative evidence. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 47(1), 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1515/ppb-2016-0011.

  • Singh, L., Fu, C. S., Rahman, A. A., Hameed, W. B., Sanmugam, S., & Agarwal, P. G. R. T. (2014). Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development,86(1), 294–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., & Ganschow, L. (2012). Profiles of more and less successful L2 learners: A cluster analysis study. Learning and Individual Differences,22, 463–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.283.5408.1657.

  • Smith-Donald, R., Raver, C. C., Hayes, T., & Richardson, B. (2007). Preliminary construct and concurrent validity of the preschool self-regulation assessment (PSRA) for field-based study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,22(2), 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, M. D., Janus, M., Moreno, S., Astheimer, L. B., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Early stage second-language learning improves executive control: Evidence from ERP. Brain and Language,139, 84–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y., & Posner, M. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,13(5), 222–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology,2, 123. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism,21, 197–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorell, L. B., & Wahlstedt, C. (2006). Executive functioning deficits in relation to symptoms of ADHD and/or ODD in preschool children. Infant and Child Development,15(5), 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychological Research,78(6), 803–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Xue, G., Chen, C., & Dong, Q. (2007). Neural bases of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: An ER-fMRI study. Neuroimage,35, 862–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science,316, 1159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology,102(1), 43–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016738.

  • Yoshida, H., Tran, D. N., Benitez, V., & Kuwabara, M. (2011). Inhibition and adjective learning in bilingual and monolingual children. Frontiers in Psychology,13(2), 210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development,11, 37–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2011). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 574–603). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dorota E. Campfield .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Campfield, D.E. (2021). Cognition and Second Language Experience: How Are Executive Function and Second Language Acquisition Related?. In: Rokita-Jaśkow, J., Wolanin, A. (eds) Facing Diversity in Child Foreign Language Education. Second Language Learning and Teaching. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-66022-2_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-66022-2_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-66021-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-66022-2

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics