Skip to main content

Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes

  • Chapter
Book cover Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation

Abstract

There is increasing evidence that automatic mental processes contribute to self-regulation failures such as eating high-fat foods despite being on a diet and getting trapped in old ways of thinking about problems that require a novel response. Mindfulness meditation, which was developed to overcome habitual patterns of the mind that contribute to human suffering, holds great promise as a strategy to improve self-regulation. This chapter examines the idea that mindfulness may facilitate self-regulation by influencing automatic processes and their relation with subsequent cognition and behavior. After beginning with a discussion of the functional value of automatic processes, the chapter continues with a review of how these processes contribute to self-regulation failure. Next, a theoretical discussion is presented concerning mindfulness and how it might moderate automatic processes. After presenting a review of research addressing this topic, the chapter concludes with suggestions for future work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    MN is the standard abbreviation for the canonical Majjhima Nikāya (“Middle-length discourses”) text.

  2. 2.

    I follow Vervaeke’s (2011) argument regarding the necessity of frames and their costs.

  3. 3.

    This sketch is abstracted from more comprehensive accounts (Bodhi, 1984; Carrithers, 1983; Goldstein, 1993; Rahula, 1959).

References

  • Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 14, 449–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553–573.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation and the executive function: The self as controlling agent. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 516–539). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 108, 652–661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., . . . Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodhi, B. (1984). The noble eightfold path: Way to the end of suffering. Onalaska, WA: Pariyatti Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? A canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 19–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, S., Chawla, N., Collins, S. E., Witkiewitz, K., Hsu, S., Grow, J., . . . Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for substance use disorders: A pilot efficacy trial. Substance Abuse, 30, 295–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breslin, F. C., Zack, M., & McMain, S. (2002). An information-processing analysis of mindfulness: Implications for relapse prevention in the treatment of substance abuse. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 275–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., . . . Rounsaville, B. J. (2011). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 119, 72–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carels, R. A., Hoffman, J., Collins, A., Raber, A. C., Cacciapaglia, H., & O’Brien, W. H. (2001). Ecological momentary assessment of temptation and lapse in dieting. Eating Behaviors, 2, 307–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrithers, M. (1983). Buddha: A very short introduction. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: The impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 116.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12, 163–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darkes, J., & Goldman, M. S. (1993). Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: Experimental evidence for a mediational process. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 344–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2003a). On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 353–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2003b). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 189–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degner, J. (2009). On the (un-)controllability of affective priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can possibly be prevented. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 327–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1984). Cognitive wheels: The frame problem of artificial intelligence. In C. Hookaway (Ed.), Minds, machines and evolution (pp. 129–151). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, A., Ulmer, L., Mutschler, J., Herre, H., Krumm, B., Croissant, B., . . . Kiefer, F. (2010). Why is disulfiram superior to acamprosate in the routine clinical setting? A retrospective long-term study in 353 alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 45, 271–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 38–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, L. M., Tiffany, S. T., Martin, E. M., & Baker, T. B. (1983). Aversive smoking therapies: A conditioning analysis of therapeutic effectiveness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 595–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, J. H., Marshall, E. J., & Peters, T. J. (2000). Outcome after in-patient detoxification for alcohol dependence: A naturalistic comparison of 7 versus 28 days stay. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 35, 580–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gethin, R. (2011). On some definitions of mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 263–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B. (2008). Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature brands? new evidence from the implicit association test. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 178–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non-insight problems. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 279–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, J. (1993). Insight meditation: The practice of freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, J. (2002). One dharma. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Reiner, K., & Meiran, N. (2012). “Mind the trap”: Mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS One, 7, e36206.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grilo, C. M., Masheb, R. M., & Wilson, G. T. (2005). Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy and fluoxetine for the treatment of binge eating disorder: A randomized double-blind placebo-controlled comparison. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 301–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gunaratana, B. H. (2002). Mindfulness in plain English. Somerville, MA: Wisdom Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169–183.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 162–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Castelli, L., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Implicit and explicit attitudes and interracial interaction: The moderating role of situationally available control resources. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11, 69–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands, G. J., Prestwich, A., & Marteau, T. M. (2011). Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychology, 30, 195–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 537–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Houben, K., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Getting a grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science, 22, 968–975.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, W. A., Barnett, L. W., & Branch, L. G. (1971). Relapse rates in addiction programs. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 455–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 109–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are. New York, NY: Hyperion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 533–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2001). Training for insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-third annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 489–493). Mahwah, NJ: Lance Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1998). The search for meaning in evolutionary perspective and its clinical implications. In P. T. P. Wong & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 27–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1534–1555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristeller, J., Wolever, R. Q., & Sheets, V. (2014). Mindfulness-based eating awareness training (mb-eat) for binge eating: A randomized clinical trial. Mindfulness, 5, 282–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Sylvain, C., Boutin, C., Lachance, S., Doucet, C., Leblond, J., & Jacques, C. (2001). Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 774–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Ricard, M. (2012). Meditation and the startle response: A case study. Emotion, 12, 650–658.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Löw, A., Lang, P. J., Smith, J. C., & Bradley, M. M. (2008). Both predator and prey: Emotional arousal in threat and reward. Psychological Science, 19, 865–873.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54, 1–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163–169.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt, G. A. (1994). Addiction, mindfulness, and acceptance. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, & M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 175–197). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1996). The human infants’ response to vanilla flavors in mother’s milk and formula. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 13–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 654–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyers, B. (Producer). (1998). Portrait of addiction. Retrieved from http://billmoyers.com/content/moyers-on-addiction-close-to-home/

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 467–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norcross, J. C., Ratzin, A. C., & Payne, D. (1989). Ringing in the new year: The change processes and reported outcomes of resolutions. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 205–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, C. P., Childress, A. R., Ehrman, R., & Robbins, S. J. (1998). Conditioning factors in drug use: Can they explain compulsion? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12, 15–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 12, 413–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Trait inferences as a function of automatically activated racial attitudes and motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Bauer, C., & Myxter, P. (2012). Mindfulness decouples the relation between automatic alcohol motivation and heavy drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31, 729–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Friese, M. (2014). Mindfulness training moderates the relation between an implicit measure of race attitude and interracial behavior. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Kassman, K. T. (2012). Stepping out of history: Mindfulness improves insight problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1031–1036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Kassman, K. T., & Wessel, I. (2013). Breaking the cycle of desire: Mindfulness and executive control weaken the relation between an implicit measure of alcohol valence and preoccupation with alcohol-related thoughts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 1153–1158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (2008). Surfing the urge: Experiential acceptance moderates the relation between automatic alcohol motivation and hazardous drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27, 404–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Marlatt, G. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Drinking without thinking: An implicit measure of alcohol motivation predicts failure to control alcohol use. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1210–1219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Verwoerd, J., & Wessel, I. (2014). Untying the knot of fear: Mindfulness training weakens attentional bias toward trauma-film stimuli. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. D. (2003). Alcohol-related motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers: Assessing implicit response tendencies using the modified-IAT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1149–1162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Custers, R. (2012). Mindful attention prevents mindless impulses. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 291–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Luo, J., & Fernández, G. (2009). Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 25–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rahula, W. (1959). What the Buddha taught. New York, NY: Grove.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raio, C. M., Orederu, T. A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 15139–15144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, R. I. (1982). Search heuristics of chess players of different calibers. The American Journal of Psychology, 95, 383–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rieskamp, J., & Otto, P. E. (2006). SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 207–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenmakers, T. M., de Bruin, M., Lux, I. F. M., Goertz, A. G., Van Kerkhof, Dorieke H. A. T., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 109, 30–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenmakers, T., Wiers, R. W., Jones, B. T., Bruce, G., & Jansen, A. T. M. (2007). Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction, 102, 399–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E. (1950). Programming a computer for playing chess. Philosophical Magazine, 41, 256–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 373–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shurick, A. A., Hamilton, J. R., Harris, L. T., Roy, A. K., Gross, J. J., & Phelps, E. A. (2012). Durable effects of cognitive restructuring on conditioned fear. Emotion, 12, 1393–1397.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1972). Theories of bounded rationality. In B. McGuire & R. Radner (Eds.), Decisions and organization (pp. 161–176). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., & Simon, P. A. (1962). Trial and error search in solving difficult problems: Evidence from the game of chess. Behavioral Science, 7, 425–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, R., Shaham, Y., & Heilig, M. (2011). Translational and reverse translational research on the role of stress in drug craving and relapse. Psychopharmacology, 218, 69–82.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H. A., Lutz A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biology, 5, 138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: The orienting reflex. Annual Reviews of Physiology, 25, 545–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & West, R. F. (1981). A longitudinal study of the development of automatic recognition skills in first graders. Journal of Reading Behavior, 13, 57–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strick, M., van Noorden, T. H. J., Ritskes, R. R., de Ruiter, J. R., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Zen meditation and access to information in the unconscious. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1476–1481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sucitto, A. (2010). Turning the wheel of truth: Commentary on the Buddha’s first teaching. Boston, MA: Shambhala.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Chaskalson, M. (2011). How does mindfulness transform suffering? II: The transformation of dukkha. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 103–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Triesch, J., Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., & Sullivan, B. T. (2003). What you see is what you need. Journal of Vision, 3, 86–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 296.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gucht, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Van, D. B., & Beckers, T. (2008). Conditioned craving cues elicit an automatic approach tendency. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1160–1169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vervaeke, J. (2011, March). Can foolishness lead to mental illness? Mind matters: Towards a new understanding of psychopathology. Lecture conducted from the University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada. Retrieved from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EE5xNA3yHYc&list=PLFDA9061A3FAC3E7D&index=1

    Google Scholar 

  • Wild elephants raid fields in southern Vietnam (2013). Thanh Nien news. Retrieved from http://www.thanhniennews.com/2010/pages/20130215-wild-elephants-damage-crops-in-southern-vietnam-during-tet.aspx

  • von Uexküll, J. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept (pp. 5–80). New York, NY: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, M., & Greaney, J. (2014). Insight problem solving is improved by mindfulness but not by relaxation. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual responding. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 21, 33–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22, 490–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 748–762.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review, 114, 843–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1281–1297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian D. Ostafin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ostafin, B.D. (2015). Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes. In: Ostafin, B., Robinson, M., Meier, B. (eds) Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics