Skip to main content
Log in

Processing speed of dyslexia: the relationship between temporal processing and rapid naming in Chinese

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We consider auditory and visual temporal processing and alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric rapid naming to separately examine the associations of temporal processing and rapid naming with Chinese character reading in children with and without dyslexia. A total of 112 Chinese children in primary school from third grade to sixth grade were recruited from Taiwan. Half of them were identified as dyslexic, and the other half were typically developing children matched to the dyslexic group by age, gender, and IQ. The interaction of groups of participants and non-alphanumeric rapid naming contributed significantly to Chinese character reading in addition to temporal processing and rapid naming. Moreover, the important role of rapid naming in the contribution of temporal processing to Chinese character reading was found only in children with dyslexia (specific to non-alphanumeric rapid naming). On the contrary, the significant contributions of temporal processing to Chinese character reading without rapid naming were found in children with and without dyslexia. However, the auditory modality was identified for typically developing children, while the visual modality was identified for children with dyslexia. The present study is among the first to show the associations among temporal processing on visual and auditory modalities, different rapid naming, and reading in Chinese children with and without dyslexia. Our results imply qualitative differences between the cognitive profiles of Chinese children with and without dyslexia. Our findings in this study may enhance understanding and preliminarily bridge the current research gap in the field of temporal processing in Chinese.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47(1), 69–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, C., Elliott, M. A., & Lachmann, T. (2005). Evidence for impaired visuoperceptual organisation in developmental dyslexics and its relation to temporal processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 499–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (1996). Auditory temporal processing thresholds, habituation, and recognition memory over the 1st year. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(3), 339–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bexkens, A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Tijms, J. (2015). Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia, 21, 212–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., de Smedt, B., & Ghesquiere, P. (2008). Modelling relations between sensory processing, speech perception, orthographic and phonological ability, and literacy achievement. Brain and Language, 106(1), 29–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high-risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1608–1620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Perfetti, C. A., MacWhinney, B., & Hunt, S. B. (2000). The association of rapid temporal perception with orthographic and phonological processing in children and adults with reading impairment. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 101–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P., Golden, J., Kennedy, A., & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 1: Theoretical and developmental issues. Neuropsychology and cognition (Vol. 8, pp. 173–218). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, L., & Holmes, V. M. (2003). The relationship between auditory temporal processing, phonemic awareness, and reading disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 218–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Misra, M. (2003). Speed of processing of the visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological systems in adult dyslexics: The contribution of “asynchrony” to word recognition deficits. Brain and Language, 85, 486–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C., & Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 27–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91(1), 77–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 510–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2007). Prevalence, gender ratio and gender differences in reading-related cognitive abilities among Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Educational Studies, 33, 249–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, J. H., & Chen, H. Y. (2006). Raven’s progressive matrices-parallel manual. Taipei, Taiwan: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W. L., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. S. H. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 15–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Cutting, L. E. (1999). History and significance of rapid automatized naming. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 29–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, W. (2007). A developmental study of phonological activation in Chinese character recognition. Master’s Thesis. University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR.

  • Ding, Y., Richman, L. C., Yang, L. Y., & Guo, J. P. (2010). Rapid automatized naming and immediate memory functions in Chinese mandarin-speaking elementary readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donker, M., Kroesbergen, E., Slot, E., Van Viersen, S., & De Bree, E. (2016). Alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric rapid automatized naming in children with reading and/or spelling difficulties and mathematical difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 80–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, B., Hartnegg, K., & Mokler, A. (2000). Dynamic visual perception of dyslexic children. Perception, 29, 523–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K. (2010). PASS cognitive processes: Can they explain the RAN-reading relationship? Psychological Science (Chinese), 33, 1291–1298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Aro, M., Liao, C.-H., & Parrila, R. (2015). The contribution of RAN pause time and articulation time to reading across languages: Evidence from a more representative sample of children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19, 135–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • González-Valenzuela, M. J., Díaz-Giráldez, F., & López-Montiel, M. D. (2016). Cognitive predictors of word and pseudoword reading in Spanish first-grade children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: An overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373–2399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, S. I., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I., Bibok, M. B., & Liebermann-Finestone, D. P. (2012). The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers’ executive function. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 271–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H. (2010). Understanding reading disability in Chinese: From basic research to intervention. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), Handbook of Chinese psychology (2nd ed., pp. 109–121). New York: Oxford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Yau, P. W.-Y., & Au, A. (2003). Development of orthographic knowledge and its relationship with reading and spelling among Chinese kindergarten and primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 51–71). London, UK: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, M., & Conlon, E. (2004). Visual and auditory temporal processing and early reading development. Dyslexia, 10, 234–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S. (2001). 中文年級認字量表 (Chinese character recognition test). Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Publishing Co Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hung, L. Y. (2002). 國小學童漢字視知覺能力三年縱貫研究 (A three-year longitudinal study of elementary students’ visual perception of Chinese characters). Bulletin of Special Education, 22, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hung, L. Y. (2006). 部件辨識測驗 (Radical recognition test). Taipei, Taiwan: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hung, L. Y., & Fang, J. Y. (2006a). 部首表義測驗 (Semantic radical test). Taipei, Taiwan: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hung, L. Y., & Fang, J. Y. (2006b). 聲旁表音測驗 (Phonetic radical test). Taipei, Taiwan: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., Hall, L. K., & Caskey, B. J. (1999). Processing speed, exposure to print, and naming speed. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (2002). Observations on the temporal correlates of reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruk, R. S., Mayer, J., & Funk, L. (2014). The predictive relations between non-alphanumeric rapid naming and growth in regular and irregular word decoding in at-risk readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 37, 17–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Willburger, E. (2010). Temporal processing, attention, and learning disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 393–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 525–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, D., Chen, X., & Chung, K. K. H. (2015). Performance in a visual search task uniquely predicts reading abilities in third-grade Hong Kong Chinese children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19, 307–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, D., Chen, X., & Wang, Y. (2016). The impact of visual-spatial attention on reading and spelling in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 1435–1447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2017). Cognitive and environmental correlates of rapid automatized naming in Chinese kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 465–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1996). Models of speech perception and phonological processing in reading. Child Development, 67, 1836–1856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W. Y., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Cheng-Lai, A., Zeng, B., Stein, J. F., & Zhou, X. (2011). Dynamic visual perception and reading development in Chinese school children. Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 161–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Sai, X., Wang, C., Wang, J., Sha, S., & Zhou, X. (2005). Auditory and speech processing and reading development in Chinese school children: Behavioural and ERP evidence. Dyslexia, 11, 292–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A., & Felton, R. H. (1998). Selective predictive value of rapid automatized naming in poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 106–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, K., Hulme, C., Nag, S., & Snowling, M. J. (2015). Sentence repetition as a marker of language skills in children with dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 203–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naples, A. J., Chang, J. T., Katz, L., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Same or different? Insights into the etiology of phonological awareness and rapid naming. Biological Psychology, 80, 226–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narhi, V., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Leppasaari, T., Korhonen, T., Tolvanen, A., et al. (2005). Rapid serial naming: Relations between different stimuli and neuropsychological factors. Brain and Language, 92, 45–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., Yan, M., Laubrock, J., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2013). Eye-voice span during rapid automatized naming of digits and dice in Chinese normal and dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 16, 967–979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures forestimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 717–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2001). Dyslexia: Talk of two theories. Nature, 412(6845), 393–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1999). The role of phonological awareness, speech perception, and auditory temporal processing for dyslexia. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, S28–S34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. A., Pennington, B. F., Yerys, B. E., Scott, A., Boada, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2006). Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 584–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (2002). Temporal processing and reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 151–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. JAMA, 264, 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, N. C., Hittner, J. B., & May, K. (2004). Testing dependent correlations with nonoverlapping variables: A Monte Carlo simulation. Journal of Experimental Education, 73, 53–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. T. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since HM. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 259–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbrink, C., Zimmer, K., Lachmann, T., Dirichs, M., & Kammer, T. (2014). Development of rapid temporal processing and its impact on literacy skills in primary school children. Child Development, 85, 1711–1726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stringer, R., & Stanovich, K. E. (2000). The connection between reaction time and variation in reading ability: Unravelling covariance relationships with cognitive ability and phonological sensitivity. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 41–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading: Some perceptual prerequisites. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 170–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (2000). Experimental studies of language learning impairments: From research to remediation. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 131–155). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and spelling errors: Keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 426–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, S. J., Chang, Y. J., Chien, S. L., & Ling, Y. T. (2011). 快速自動化唸名測驗 (Rapid automatized naming test). Taipei, Taiwan: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, S. J., Chien, S. L., & Hsieh, Y. C. (2006). 聲韻覺識測驗—篩選 (Phonological awareness test battery—screening). Taipei, Taiwan: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J. H., & Lutje Spelberg, H. C. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors, and pictured objects, and word-reading speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 25–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J., & van den Broeck, W. (2003). Specific relations between alphanumeric-naming speed and reading speeds of monosyllabic and multisyllabic words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 407–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L. C., & Yang, H. M. (2014). Classifying Chinese children with dyslexia by dual-route and triangle models of Chinese reading. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 2702–2713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L.-C., & Yang, H.-M. (2018). Temporal processing development in Chinese primary school-aged children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(3), 302–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (2000). Naming-speed processes and developmental reading disabilities: An introduction to the special issue on the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, W. G., & Weekes, B. S. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese: Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 255–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zettler, C. M. (2007). Timing variables in reading and language: The relation of naming speed and motor speed to auditory temporal processing. Doctoral dissertation. Georgia State University.

  • Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2014). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, L1 Chinese, and L2 English reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1001–1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Li-Chih Wang.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, LC., Liu, D., Chen, JK. et al. Processing speed of dyslexia: the relationship between temporal processing and rapid naming in Chinese. Read Writ 31, 1645–1668 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9857-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9857-2

Keywords

Navigation