Skip to main content
Log in

Activating Basic Category Exemplars in Sentence Contexts: A Dynamical Account

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines the influence of context on the processing of category names embedded in sentences. The investigation focuses on the nature of information available immediately after such a word is heard as well as on the dynamics of adaptation to context. An on-line method (Cross Modal Lexical Priming) was used to trace how this process unfolds in time. We found that the information available immediately after a category word is presented is not altered by the sentence context in which the word is immersed. Rather, the structure of availability of particular exemplars of the category resembles the typicality structure of a conceptual representation. The adaptation to context occurs later (between 300 and 450 ms after the category word) and takes the form of a rapid reorganization of the structure rather than a gradual activation of a contextually relevant exemplar. We claim that such data is best accounted for in a dynamical framework, where a coherent global structure emerges through locally guided self-organization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson R.C., Pichert J.W., Goetz E.T., Schallert D.L., Stevens K.V., Trollip S.R. (1976). Instantiation of general terms. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 15(6): 667–679

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson R.C., Shiffrin Z. (1980). The meaning of words in context. In: Spiro R.J., Bruice B.C., Brewer W.F., (eds). Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Earlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 331–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou L.W. (1982). Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. Memory and Cognition 20, 82–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou L.W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition 11(3): 211–227

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou L.W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In: U. Neisser (eds). Concepts and conceptual development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 101–140

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwise J., Perry J. (1983). Situations and attitudes. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Borsky S., Tuller B., Shapiro L.P. (1998). How to milk a coat: The effects of semantic and acoustic information on phoneme categorization. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103, 2670–2676

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner J.S. (1973). Beyond the information given: Studies in the psychology of knowing. New York, Norton

    Google Scholar 

  • Case P., Tuller B., Ding M., Kelso J.A.S. (1995). Evaluation of a dynamical model of speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics 57(7): 977–988

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins A.M., Quillian M.R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8, 240–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cree G.S., McRae K., McNorgan C. (1999). An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: Simulating semantic priming. Cognitive Science 23(3): 371–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cussins A. (1990). The connectionist construction of concepts. In: Boden M.A., (eds). The philosophy of artificial intelligence. Oxford readings in philosophy. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 368—440

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy S.A., Morris R.K., Rayner K. (1988). Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language 27, 429–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J.A. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster K.I. (1979). Levels of processing and the structure of language processor. In: Cooper W., Walker E., (eds). Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies. Earlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster K.I. (1985). Lexical acquisition and the modular lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 1(2): 87–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis W.N., Kucera H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston, Houghton Mifflin

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujihara N., Nageishi Y., Koyama S., Nakajima Y. (1998). Electrophysiological evidence for the typicality effect of human cognitive categorization. International Journal of Psychophysiology 29, 65–75

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haken H. (1983). Synergetics, an introduction: Non-equilibrium phase transitions and self-organization in physics, chemistry and biology (3rd edn). Berlin, Springer

    Google Scholar 

  • Haken H. (1990). Synergetics as a tool for the conceptualization and mathematization of cognition and behavior–How far can we go?. In: Haken H., Stadler M., (eds). Synergetics of cognition. Springer, Berlin, pp. 2—31

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampton J.A. (1998). Similarity based categorization and fuzziness of natural categories. Cognition 56, 137–165

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hock H., Kelso J.A.S., Schöner G. (1993). Bistability, hysteresis and loss of temporal stability in the perceptual organization of apparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19(1): 63–80

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfield J.J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 81, 6871–6875

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeka J.J., Kelso J.A.S. (1989). The dynamic approach to coordinated behavior: A tutorial review. In: Wallace S.A., (eds). Perspectives on the coordination of movement. Elsevier Science Publishers B.V, North Holland

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawamoto A.H. (1993). Nonlinear dynamics in the resolution of lexical ambiguity: A parallel distributed processing account. Journal of Memory and Language 32, 474–516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso J.A.S. (1990). Phase transitions: Foundations of behavior. In: Haken H., Stadler M., (eds). Synergetics of cognition. Springer, Berlin, pp. 249—268

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso J.A.S. (1995). Dynamic patterns. MIT press, Cambridge MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso J.A.S., Case P., Holroyd T., Horvath E., Rączaszek J., Tuller B., Ding M. (1994). Multistability and metastability in perceptual and brain dynamics. In: Kruse P., Stadler M., (eds). Ambiguity in mind and nature. Berlin, Springer-Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso J.A.S., Scholz J.P., Schöner G. (1986). Non-equilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical fluctuations. Physics Letters A118, 279–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, A. G., & Anderson, J.A. Theory of categorization based on distributed memory storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 10(4): 616–637

  • Łozowski P. (2000). Vagueness in language. Lublin, UMCS Press

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald M.C., Pearlmutter N.J., Seidenberg M.S. (1994). Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101(4): 676–703

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson W., Tyler L.K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition 8, 1–71

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson W., Tyler L.K. (1987). Against modularity. In: Garfield J.L. (eds). Modularity in knowledge representation and natural-language understanding. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 37–62

    Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy C.L., Nelson D.L. (1982). Category name and instance norms for 106 categories of various sizes. American Journal of Psychology 95, 581–634

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae K., de Sa V., Seidenberg M.S. (1997). On nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126, 99–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Wattenmaker, W. D. (1987). Category cohesiveness, theories, and cognitive archeology. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Emory symposia in cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 25–62). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

  • Mervis C.B., Rosch E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology 32, 89–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neely J.H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 106, 226–254

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicol J., Swinney D., Love T., Hald L. (2006). The on-line study of sentence processing: examination of dual-task paradigms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 35(3): 215–231

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onifer W., Swinney D. (1981). Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory and Cognition 9(3): 225–236

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner M.I., KeeleS.W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77(3): 353–363

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rączaszek J. (1996). A dynamic view of the processing of ambiguous expressions. Polish Psychological Bulletin 27, 189–199

    Google Scholar 

  • Rączaszek J., Tuller B., Shapiro L.P., Case P., Kelso J.A.S. (1999). Categorization of ambiguous sentences as a function of a changing prosodic parameter: A dynamical approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28(4): 367–393

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124, 372–422

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rips L.J., Smith E.E., Shoben E.J. (1978). Semantic composition in sentence verification. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 17(4): 375–401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 103, 192–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch E. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8(3): 382–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth E.M., Shoben E.J. (1983). The effect of context on the structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 15, 346–378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & the PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

  • Schöner G., Kelso J.A.S. (1988). Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239, 1513–1520

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber A., Rousset S., Tiberghien G. (1990). Context effects in face recognition: Below response bias. The contribution of a simulation. In: Caverni J.-P., Fabre J.-M., Gonzalez M., (eds). Cognitive biases. Advances in psychology. North-Holland, Amsterdam, Netherlands, pp. 243–274

  • Seidenberg M.S., Tannenhaus M.K., Leiman J.M., Bienkowski M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology 14, 489–537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro L.P., Hestvik A., Lesan L., Garcia A.R. (2003). Charting the time-course of sentence processing: Evidence for an initial and independent structural analysis. Journal of Memory and Language 49, 1–19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro L.P., Swinney D.A., Borsky S. (1998). On-line examination of language performance in normal and neurologically-impaired adults. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 7, 49–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson G.B. (1981). Meaning dominance and semantic context in the processing of lexical ambiguity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 120–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson G.B. (1984). Lexical ambiguity and its role in models of word recognition. Psychological Bulletin 96, 316–340

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swaab T., Brown C., Hagoort P. (2003). Understanding words in sentence contexts: The time course of ambiguity resolution. Brain and Language 86, 326–343

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swinney D.A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 18(6): 645–659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swinney D., Osterhout L. (1990). Inference generation during auditory language comprehension. In: Graesser A.C., Bower G.H., (eds). Inference and text comprehension: The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 25). San Diego: Academic Press,

  • Tabor W., Galantucci B., Richardson D. (2003). Effects of merely local syntactic coherence on sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language 50, 355–370

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabor W., Hutchins S. (2004). Evidence for self-organized sentence processing: Digging-in effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30, 431–450

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi P. (1988). Accessing lexical ambiguity in different types of sentential contexts. Journal of Memory and Language 27, 324–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannenhaus M.K., Lucas M.M. (1987). Context effects in lexical processing. Cognition 25, 213–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuller B., Case P., Kelso J.A.S., Ding M. (1994). The nonlinear dynamics of categorical perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20(1):3–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tuller B., Kelso J.A.S. (1990). Phase transitions in speech production and their perceptual consequences. In: Jeannerod M., (eds). Attention and performance, XIII. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 429–452

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey M.T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist 45, 938–953

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitney P. (1986). Processing category terms in context: Instantiations as inferences. Memory and Cognition 14(1): 39–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitney P., Kellas (1984). Processing category terms in context: instantiation and the structure of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 10: 95–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitney P., McKay T., Kellas G., Emerson W.A. (1985). Semantic activation of noun concepts in context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 11(1): 126–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widrow, G., & Hoff, M. E. (1960). Adaptive switching circuits. In Institute of Radio Engineers, Western Electronic Show and Convention, Convention Record (Vol. 4, pp. 96–104).

  • Wittgenstein L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford, B. Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joanna Rączaszek-Leonardi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., Shapiro, L.P., Tuller, B. et al. Activating Basic Category Exemplars in Sentence Contexts: A Dynamical Account. J Psycholinguist Res 37, 87–113 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-007-9061-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-007-9061-z

Keywords

Navigation