Skip to main content
Log in

Artificial grammar learning in children: abstraction of rules or sensitivity to perceptual features?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined sensitivity to grammatical sequences of colors in an artificial grammar learning task in a sample of 120 children aged between 5 and 8 years. The aim of the experiment was to test whether the children would preferentially learn the specific salient features of the items they were exposed to or the rules that generated these items. The children were divided into two experimental groups (identical grammar but training items differing in their surface features) and a control group (random items). The results showed that regardless of age, participants learned the most frequent salient features of the items, as well as some kind of abstract relational information. However, the 8-year-olds presented a more complex result profile, with one of the experimental groups apparently developing sensitivity to grammatical rules. These results are discussed with reference to the main current models of implicit learning. Overall, the results provided more support for stimulus-specific processing models than for rule-based models.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We sincerely thank an anonymous reviewer for this nice metaphor and argument.

  2. It may be worth explaining why it was not possible to include another test (such as a recognition test, for instance) following the implicit generation test. Recognition is very likely to elicit intentional information recollection processes (Gardiner & Java, 1993), with the result that such a test is a much more explicit test than an implicit generation test. Several authors have pointed out the danger of contamination between implicit and explicit tests when they are jointly administered to the same participants (Gebauer & Mackintosh, 2007; Seger et al., 2000; Shimamura, 1985). Counterbalancing the order between tests does not resolve the problem. A recognition test cannot be administered first because half of the items presented during this test are non-grammatical. However, exposing participants to errors (non-grammatical information) is known to be prejudicial to implicit learning (e.g., Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Perruchet, et al. 2006) and this can affect performance in a subsequent test. The same reasoning holds if the generation test is presented first. Such a test inevitably leads participants to generate false (non-grammatical) items (partially or completely).

  3. This does not mean that the children could not evoke the presence of adjacent or non-adjacent repetitions. As we saw in a small number of children, if the experimenter explicitly asked "Were the flags always made of different colors, or were some colors sometimes repeated inside the flags?", they were able to recognize that some colors were sometimes repeated. The explicit knowledge that could be expressed by children in the question phase was highly dependent on the type of questions asked by the experimenter.

  4. Perruchet and Reber (2003, p. 129) argued that "if trained control subjects do not perform at chance level, this means that some undetected biases are still in operation". The below-chance production of repetitions in the random group probably expressed a natural bias against repetitions. Because the additivity assumption claims that non-specific variables have similar effects on the experimental and control groups trained in very close conditions, if repetition avoidance led to below-chance productions in the control group, it was very likely that sensitivity to repetitive structures raised performance in the experimental groups. However, this increase reached only chance level, not more. For a more direct demonstration of such learning, we computed the ratios between observed and theoretical proportions, and run an ANOVA with Type of series as a between-subjects factor. Type of series yielded significance for the non-adjacent and adjacent repetitions, F(2, 117) = 7.05, p < .01 and F(2, 117) = 18.25, p < .01 respectively. Planned comparisons showed that the non-adjacent group produced more non-adjacent repetitions than the adjacent and random groups, F(1, 117) = 9.46, p < .01 and F(1, 117) = 11.58, p < .01 respectively, as did the adjacent group for the adjacent repetitions, F(1, 117) = 26.85, p < .01 and F(1, 117) = 27.9, p < .01. Thus, there was a specific increase of adjacent or non-adjacent repetitions only in the groups where specific learning was expected.

  5. The results suggest that the frequent initial bigrams were not learned, as their mean frequency was very close to the theoretical probability. However, it is worth pointing out that the second color in these bigrams was the first color of the repeated adjacent unit (e.g., BYY). Learning the initial bigram (BY) may have been prevented, or at least slowed down, by the formation of the repeated unit (YY). It would appear to be very interesting to study this issue in greater detail because it reveals the limitations of a purely statistical view of learning.

References

  • Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 53–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 316–323.

  • Carr, T. H., & Curran, T. (1994). Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences: Applications to syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 205–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, S. (1993). Are rules and modules really necessary for explaining language? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 593–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. New York: Psychology Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Endress, A. D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Mehler, J. (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition, 105, 577–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1972). Cognitive mechanisms in autism: Experiments with color and tone sequence production. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 160–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. (2000). The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1179–1191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1993). Recognising and remembering. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 163–188). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 34–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, R. L., & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70(2), 109–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, L. (Ed). (2003). Attention and implicit learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

  • Johansson, T. (2009). Strengthening the case for stimulus-specificity in artificial grammar learning. Experimental Psychology, 56, 188–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 169–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krist, H., Fieberg, E. L., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 952–966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ling, J., & Blades, M. (1996). Incidental recall of color information by children and adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ling, J., & Blades, M. (2002). Further evidence for incidental encoding of color by children and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 537–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning (pp. 73–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, R. C., Buss, R. R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchards-Fields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit process in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1083–1100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meulemans, T., & Van der Linden, M. (1997). Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1007–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittenecker, E. (1958). Analysis of sequences of “chance” action. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 5, 45–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monaghan, P., & Rowson, C. (2008). The effect of repetition and similarity on sequence learning. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1509–1514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning in real world context: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, H., Blades, M., & Andrade, J. (1999). Children’s incidental recall of color information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 41A, 797–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P. (1994). Defining the knowledge units of a synthetic language: Comment on Vokey and Brooks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 223–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Reber, R. (2003). Why untrained control groups provide invalid baselines: A reply to Dienes & Altmann. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 125–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., Rey, A., Hivert, E., & Pacton, S. (2006). Do distractors interfere with memory for study pairs in associative recognition? Memory & Cognition, 34, 1046–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (1998). Learning and development. The implicit knowledge assumption reconsidered. In M. Stadler & P. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 495–531). Thousands Oaks: Sage publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (2002). The Self-Organizing Consciousness (Target paper). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picard, D., & Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children’s drawings: Effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poletiek, F. H., & van Schijndel, T. J. P. (2009). Stimulus set size and statistical coverage of the grammar in artificial grammar learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1058–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redington, M., & Chater, N. (2002). Knowledge representation and transfer in artificial grammar learning. In R. A. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning. Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seger, C. A., Prabhakaran, V., Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Neural activity differs between explicit and implicit learning of artificial grammar strings: An fMRI study. Psychology, 28, 283–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (1985). Problems with the finding of stochastic independence as evidence for multiple memory systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., Langston, C., & Nisbett, R. E. (1992). The case of rules in reasoning. Cognitive science, 16(1), 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M., & Frensch, P. (Eds.) (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousands Oaks: Sage Publications.

  • Tunney, R. J., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2001). Two modes of transfer in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 614–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Marot, V. (2007). The development of context sensitivity in children’s graphic copying strategies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 94–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (1999). Isolating unconscious influences: The neutral parameter procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 857–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Implicit motor learning through observational training in adults and children. Memory & Cognition, 30, 256–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vokey, J. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1992). Salience of item knowledge in learning artificial grammars. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 328–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiegersma, S. (1982). A control theory of sequential response production. Psychological Research, 44, 175–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, A., & Vinter, A. (in press). Learning implicitly to produce avoided behaviours. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/1747.0218.2010.543

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors are very grateful to Stéphane Argon and Laurent Bergerot, who designed and programmed the video game. We thank Pierre Perruchet and Paul Molin very much for their precious help in the programming of the algorithms for computing these theoretical proportions, and Pierre Perruchet for his very constructive comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. We are also grateful to the Inspection Académique de Dijon that gave us the opportunity to run our experiments inside some kindergarten and primary grade schools. This research was supported by a grant from the Conseil Régional de Bourgogne.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annie Vinter.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Witt, A., Vinter, A. Artificial grammar learning in children: abstraction of rules or sensitivity to perceptual features?. Psychological Research 76, 97–110 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0328-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0328-5

Keywords

Navigation